Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem

dc.contributor.author
Woolgar, Alexandra  
dc.contributor.author
Duncan, John  
dc.contributor.author
Manes, Facundo Francisco  
dc.contributor.author
Fedorenko, Evelina  
dc.date.available
2020-03-12T20:22:03Z  
dc.date.issued
2018-03  
dc.identifier.citation
Woolgar, Alexandra; Duncan, John; Manes, Facundo Francisco; Fedorenko, Evelina; Fluid intelligence is supported by the multiple-demand system not the language system; Nature Publishing Group; Nature Human Behaviour; 2; 3; 3-2018; 200-204  
dc.identifier.issn
2397-3374  
dc.identifier.uri
http://hdl.handle.net/11336/99374  
dc.description.abstract
A set of frontoparietal brain regions-the multiple-demand (MD) system 1,2-has been linked to fluid intelligence in brain imaging 3,4 and in studies of patients with brain damage 5-7 . For example, the amount of damage to frontal or parietal, but not temporal, cortices predicts fluid intelligence deficit 5 . However, frontal and parietal lobes are structurally 8 and functionally 9,10 heterogeneous. They contain domain-general regions that respond across diverse tasks 11,12, but also specialized regions that respond selectively during language processing 13 . Since language may be critical for complex thought 14-24 (compare with refs 25,26 ), intelligence loss following damage to the frontoparietal cortex could have important contributions from damage to language-selective regions. To evaluate the relative contributions of MD versus language-selective regions, we employed large functional magnetic resonance imaging datasets to construct probabilistic maps of the two systems. We used these maps to weigh the volume of lesion (in each of 80 patients) falling within each system. MD-weighted, but not language-weighted, lesion volumes predicted fluid intelligence deficit (with the opposite pattern observed for verbal fluency), indicating that fluid intelligence is specifically tied to the MD system, and undermining claims that language is at the core of complex thought.  
dc.format
application/pdf  
dc.language.iso
eng  
dc.publisher
Nature Publishing Group  
dc.rights
info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess  
dc.rights.uri
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/2.5/ar/  
dc.subject
FLUID INTELLIGENCE  
dc.subject
MULTIPLE DEMAND SYSTEM  
dc.subject.classification
Neurociencias  
dc.subject.classification
Medicina Básica  
dc.subject.classification
CIENCIAS MÉDICAS Y DE LA SALUD  
dc.title
Fluid intelligence is supported by the multiple-demand system not the language system  
dc.type
info:eu-repo/semantics/article  
dc.type
info:ar-repo/semantics/artículo  
dc.type
info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion  
dc.date.updated
2020-03-10T12:27:17Z  
dc.journal.volume
2  
dc.journal.number
3  
dc.journal.pagination
200-204  
dc.journal.pais
Estados Unidos  
dc.description.fil
Fil: Woolgar, Alexandra. Macquarie University; Australia  
dc.description.fil
Fil: Duncan, John. University of Oxford; Reino Unido. University of Cambridge; Estados Unidos  
dc.description.fil
Fil: Manes, Facundo Francisco. Macquarie University; Australia. Universidad Favaloro; Argentina. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas. Oficina de Coordinación Administrativa Houssay. Instituto de Neurociencia Cognitiva y Traslacional. Fundación Ineco Rosario Sede del Incyt | Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva. Instituto de Neurociencia Cognitiva y Traslacional. Fundación Ineco Rosario Sede del Incyt | Fundación Favaloro. Instituto de Neurociencia Cognitiva y Traslacional. Fundación Ineco Rosario Sede del Incyt; Argentina  
dc.description.fil
Fil: Fedorenko, Evelina. Harvard Medical School; Estados Unidos. Massachusetts General Hospital; Estados Unidos  
dc.journal.title
Nature Human Behaviour  
dc.relation.alternativeid
info:eu-repo/semantics/altIdentifier/doi/http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0282-3  
dc.relation.alternativeid
info:eu-repo/semantics/altIdentifier/url/https://www.nature.com/articles/s41562-017-0282-3