Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem
dc.contributor.author
Cardona Londoño, Juan Felipe
dc.contributor.author
Kargieman, Lucila
dc.contributor.author
Sinay, Vladimiro
dc.contributor.author
Gershanik, Oscar Samuel
dc.contributor.author
Gelormini Lezama, Carlos
dc.contributor.author
Amoruso, Lucía
dc.contributor.author
Roca, Martin
dc.contributor.author
Pineda, David
dc.contributor.author
Trujillo Arias, Natalia
dc.contributor.author
Michon, Maeva
dc.contributor.author
García, Adolfo Martín
dc.contributor.author
Szenkman, Daniela
dc.contributor.author
Bekinschtein, Tristán Andrés
dc.contributor.author
Manes, Facundo Francisco
dc.contributor.author
Ibañez, Agustin Mariano
dc.date.available
2018-02-01T13:20:56Z
dc.date.issued
2014-04
dc.identifier.citation
Cardona Londoño, Juan Felipe; Kargieman, Lucila; Sinay, Vladimiro; Gershanik, Oscar Samuel; Gelormini Lezama, Carlos; et al.; How embodied is action language? Neurological evidence from motor diseases; Elsevier Science; Cognition; 131; 2; 4-2014; 311-322
dc.identifier.issn
0010-0277
dc.identifier.uri
http://hdl.handle.net/11336/35348
dc.description.abstract
Although motor-language coupling is now being extensively studied, its underlying mechanisms are not fully understood. In this sense, a crucial opposition has emerged between the non-representational and the representational views of embodiment. The former posits that action language is grounded on the non-brain motor system directly engaged by musculoskeletal activity – i.e., peripheral involvement of ongoing actions. Conversely, the latter proposes that such grounding is afforded by the brain’s motor system – i.e., activation of neural areas representing motor action. We addressed this controversy through the action-sentence compatibility effect (ACE) paradigm, which induces a contextual coupling of motor actions and verbal processing. ACEs were measured in three patient groups – early Parkinson’s disease (EPD), neuromyelitis optica (NMO), and acute transverse myelitis (ATM) patients – as well as their respective healthy controls. NMO and ATM constitute models of injury to non-brain motor areas and the peripheral motor system, whereas EPD provides a model of brain motor system impairment. In our study, EPD patients exhibited impaired ACE and verbal processing relative to healthy participants, NMO, and ATM patients. These results indicate that the processing of action-related words is mainly subserved by a cortico-subcortical motor network system, thus supporting a brain-based embodied view on action language. More generally, our findings are consistent with contemporary perspectives for which action/verb processing depends on distributed brain networks supporting context-sensitive motor-language coupling.
dc.format
application/pdf
dc.language.iso
eng
dc.publisher
Elsevier Science
dc.rights
info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
dc.rights.uri
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/2.5/ar/
dc.subject
Epd
dc.subject
Nmo
dc.subject
Atm
dc.subject
Action Language
dc.subject
Representations
dc.subject
Embodied Cognition
dc.subject.classification
Inmunología
dc.subject.classification
Medicina Básica
dc.subject.classification
CIENCIAS MÉDICAS Y DE LA SALUD
dc.title
How embodied is action language? Neurological evidence from motor diseases
dc.type
info:eu-repo/semantics/article
dc.type
info:ar-repo/semantics/artículo
dc.type
info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
dc.date.updated
2018-01-31T20:06:07Z
dc.journal.volume
131
dc.journal.number
2
dc.journal.pagination
311-322
dc.journal.pais
Países Bajos
dc.journal.ciudad
Amsterdam
dc.description.fil
Fil: Cardona Londoño, Juan Felipe. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas; Argentina. Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva; Argentina. Universidad Tecnológica de Pereira; Colombia
dc.description.fil
Fil: Kargieman, Lucila. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas; Argentina. Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva; Argentina
dc.description.fil
Fil: Sinay, Vladimiro. Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva; Argentina
dc.description.fil
Fil: Gershanik, Oscar Samuel. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas; Argentina. Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva; Argentina
dc.description.fil
Fil: Gelormini Lezama, Carlos. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas; Argentina. Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva; Argentina
dc.description.fil
Fil: Amoruso, Lucía. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas; Argentina. Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva; Argentina
dc.description.fil
Fil: Roca, Martin. Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva; Argentina
dc.description.fil
Fil: Pineda, David. Universidad de Antioquia; Colombia
dc.description.fil
Fil: Trujillo Arias, Natalia. Universidad de Antioquia; Colombia
dc.description.fil
Fil: Michon, Maeva. Universidad Diego Portales; Chile
dc.description.fil
Fil: García, Adolfo Martín. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas; Argentina. Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva; Argentina
dc.description.fil
Fil: Szenkman, Daniela. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas; Argentina. Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva; Argentina
dc.description.fil
Fil: Bekinschtein, Tristán Andrés. Medical Research Council; Reino Unido
dc.description.fil
Fil: Manes, Facundo Francisco. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas; Argentina. Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva; Argentina. Australian Research Council; Australia
dc.description.fil
Fil: Ibáñez Barassi, Agustín Mariano. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas; Argentina. Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva; Argentina. Universidad Diego Portales; Chile. Universidad Autónoma del Caribe. Barranquilla; Colombia
dc.journal.title
Cognition
dc.relation.alternativeid
info:eu-repo/semantics/altIdentifier/doi/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.02.001
dc.relation.alternativeid
info:eu-repo/semantics/altIdentifier/url/https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027714000249
Archivos asociados