Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem

dc.contributor.author
Delgado, Ana R.  
dc.contributor.author
Prieto, Gerardo  
dc.contributor.author
Burin, Debora Ines  
dc.date.available
2022-06-28T16:01:55Z  
dc.date.issued
2020-08  
dc.identifier.citation
Delgado, Ana R.; Prieto, Gerardo; Burin, Debora Ines; Agreement on emotion labels’ frequency in eight Spanish linguistic areas; Public Library of Science; Plos One; 15; 8; 8-2020; 1-10  
dc.identifier.issn
1932-6203  
dc.identifier.uri
http://hdl.handle.net/11336/160649  
dc.description.abstract
Various traditions have investigated the relationship between emotion and language. For the basic emotions view, emotional prototypes are lexically sedimented in language, evidenced in cultural convergence in emotional recognition and expression tasks. For constructionist theories, conceptual knowledge supported by language is at the core of emotions. Understanding emotion words is embedded in various interrelated constructs such asemotional intelligence, emotion knowledge or emotion differentiation, and is related to, but different from, general vocabulary. A clear advantage of Emotion Vocabulary over most emotion-related constructs is that it can be measured objectively. In two successive corpus-based studies, we tested the predictions of concordance and absolute agreement on the frequency of use of a total of 100 Spanish emotion labels in the eight main Spanishspeaking areas: Spain, Mexico-Central America, River Plate, Continental Caribbean, Andean, Antilles, Chilean, and the United States. In both studies, the intraclass correlation coefficient was statistically different from the null and very large, over .95, as was the Kendall’s concordance coefficient, indicating broad consensus among the Spanish linguistic areas. From an applied perspective, our results provide supporting evidence for the similarity in frequency, and therefore cross-cultural generalizability regarding familiarity of the 100 emotion labels as item stems or as experimental stimuli without going through a process of additional adaptation. On a broader scope, these results add evidence on the role of language for emotion theories. In this regard, countries and regions compared here share the sameSpanish language, but differ in several aspects in history, culture, and socio-economic structure.  
dc.format
application/pdf  
dc.language.iso
eng  
dc.publisher
Public Library of Science  
dc.rights
info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess  
dc.rights.uri
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.5/ar/  
dc.subject
EMOTION  
dc.subject
EMOTION WORDS  
dc.subject
CULTURAL DIVERSITY  
dc.subject
WORD FREQUENCY  
dc.subject.classification
Otras Psicología  
dc.subject.classification
Psicología  
dc.subject.classification
CIENCIAS SOCIALES  
dc.title
Agreement on emotion labels’ frequency in eight Spanish linguistic areas  
dc.type
info:eu-repo/semantics/article  
dc.type
info:ar-repo/semantics/artículo  
dc.type
info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion  
dc.date.updated
2022-06-24T17:23:41Z  
dc.journal.volume
15  
dc.journal.number
8  
dc.journal.pagination
1-10  
dc.journal.pais
Estados Unidos  
dc.journal.ciudad
California  
dc.description.fil
Fil: Delgado, Ana R.. Universidad de Salamanca; España  
dc.description.fil
Fil: Prieto, Gerardo. Universidad de Salamanca; España  
dc.description.fil
Fil: Burin, Debora Ines. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas; Argentina. Universidad de Buenos Aires. Facultad de Psicología; Argentina  
dc.journal.title
Plos One  
dc.relation.alternativeid
info:eu-repo/semantics/altIdentifier/url/https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0237722  
dc.relation.alternativeid
info:eu-repo/semantics/altIdentifier/doi/http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237722