Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem

dc.contributor.author
Canet Juric, Lorena  
dc.contributor.author
Andrés, María Laura  
dc.contributor.author
García Coni Bosch, Ana Virginia  
dc.contributor.author
Richard's, Maria Marta  
dc.contributor.author
Burin, Debora Ines  
dc.date.available
2020-10-21T20:31:41Z  
dc.date.issued
2017-12  
dc.identifier.citation
Canet Juric, Lorena; Andrés, María Laura; García Coni Bosch, Ana Virginia; Richard's, Maria Marta; Burin, Debora Ines; Desempeño en memoria de trabajo e indicadores comportamentales: relaciones entre medidas directas e indirectas; Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas. Centro Interamericano de Investigaciones Psicológicas y Ciencias Afines; Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas. Centro Interdisciplinario de Investigaciones en Psicología Matemática y Experimental; Interdisciplinaria; 34; 2; 12-2017; 369-387  
dc.identifier.issn
1668-7027  
dc.identifier.uri
http://hdl.handle.net/11336/116283  
dc.description.abstract
La memoria de trabajo (MT) es un fuerte predictor del desempeño académico por su impacto en la comprensión lectora, la competencia aritmética y los comportamientos necesarios para el aprendizaje escolar. De modo que saber cómo opera la MT en niños de edad escolar y conocer la percepción que los docentes tienen sobre la MT de sus estudiantes es de gran interés. Por eso, los objetivos del trabajo que se informa fueron evaluar si las observaciones de los docentes sobre las dificultades en la MT de sus alumnos (medidas indirectas) se relacionan con el rendimiento de estos alumnos en pruebas de ejecución de MT (medidas directas) y analizar si estas dificultades percibidas por los docentes se corresponden con la presencia de problemas comportamentales de diversos tipos. Para esto se les administraron a niños de edad escolar pruebas de MT verbal y MT viso-espacial informatizadas, y a sus docentes dos cuestionarios, la Escala Observacional de Memoria de Trabajo -WMRS- y la Guía de Observación Comportamental -GOC-. Los resultados obtenidos muestran asociaciones significativas entre indicadores de fallos en MT y la performance de los niños en la tarea de MT verbal, debidas probablemente a que en los comportamientos explorados en la WMRS el componente verbal está más claramente reflejado (e.g., requiere repeticiones de instrucciones). También se encontró que estos fallos se relacionan con problemas comportamentales reportados por los docentes, como ser dificultades atencionales, hiperactividad y autoagresión. Se considera al estudio realizado como una contribución porque no hay escalas observacionales de MT para uso docente, disponibles en nuestro medio.  
dc.description.abstract
Working memory (WM) is a strong predictor of academic performance, due to its impact in reading comprehension, arithmetic’s competence and the regulated behaviors required for school learning. Thus, knowing about WM operation in school aged children and teachers’ perception on their students WM is of great value and interest. This is why in the present work we assess whether teachers’ reports about their students’ WM (indirect measure) is associated with their performance in WM tasks (direct measure), and also to assess whether the difficulties perceived by the teachers correspond to the presence of students’ general behavioral issues. For that, we administered computerized verbal WM tasks to school-aged children, in which they had to recall a number of digits (control condition) while saying out loud the color of the items (interference condition), and computerized visual-spatial WM tasks, in which they had to recall the locations of a series of stimuli (control condition) while indicating the color of the stimuli in a color palette (interference condition). We also administered two scales to their teachers: one of them asks about WM specifically (Working Memory Rating Scale) and is composed of 20 short descriptions of behavior problems that allow discriminating between children with poor and average WM skills. The teacher rates how typical a behavior is in each child, in a scale that goes from Not typical at all (0) to Very typical (3). The other asks about diverse behavioral problems (Guide of Behavioral Observation) and is composed of the following sub-scales: Physical and Verbal Aggression, Negativism, Transgression, Impulsivity, Hyperactivity, Attention Deficit and Acceptance. The teacher must choose the option that best describes the behavior in the child in terms of frequency (Never, Sometimes or Often). Our results show significant associations between children performance in the verbal WM task (direct measure) and WM failures’ indicators (indirect measures), which can be explained by the fact that the verbal component is more clearly reflected in the behaviors explored in the Working Memory Rating Scale (e.g., loses his place in complicated activities, requires regular repetition of instructions, depends on neighbor to remind them of the current task). We also wanted to look out if there were associations between the WM skills reported by the teachers and the general behavior problems also reported by them, and we found associations between the reported WM difficulties and attention disorders, hyperactivity and self-aggression. This is consistent with the claim that WM and attention deficit in nonclinical population are closely related. With respect to hyperactivity, itsrelation with WM is not as clear and has not been so frequently reported. With respect to self-aggression, its relation with WM difficulties can be attributed to these children’s inability to exercise control over the environment, which leads to frustration and low levels of motivation and self-esteem. Lastly, we found associations between children’s performance in the computerized WM tasks and the presence of behavioral problems informed by the teachers. Specifically, proper performance in verbal WM tasks associates with less attention difficulties, where as proper performance in visual-spatial WM tasks associates with being accepted by the group. This last association could be attributed to the fact that many group games require several abilities that involve visual-spatial WM, such as the ability to orientate in space and recall different locations. To conclude, we consider that this work constitutes a contribution because, in the first place, there are no WM observation scales for teachers’ use availablein our environment, and, in the second place, it addresses the need for teachers to acknowledge WM difficulties and consequently be trained for the implementation of effective strategies to improve WM skills in the classroom.  
dc.format
application/pdf  
dc.language.iso
spa  
dc.publisher
Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas. Centro Interamericano de Investigaciones Psicológicas y Ciencias Afines; Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas. Centro Interdisciplinario de Investigaciones en Psicología Matemática y Experimental  
dc.rights
info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess  
dc.rights.uri
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/2.5/ar/  
dc.subject
Memoria de Trabajo  
dc.subject
Indicadores Comportamental  
dc.subject
Percepción Docente  
dc.subject
Atención  
dc.subject
Hiperactividad  
dc.subject.classification
Otras Psicología  
dc.subject.classification
Psicología  
dc.subject.classification
CIENCIAS SOCIALES  
dc.title
Desempeño en memoria de trabajo e indicadores comportamentales: relaciones entre medidas directas e indirectas  
dc.title
Relationship between performance in working memory and behavioral indicators observed in class  
dc.type
info:eu-repo/semantics/article  
dc.type
info:ar-repo/semantics/artículo  
dc.type
info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion  
dc.date.updated
2018-10-23T14:43:37Z  
dc.journal.volume
34  
dc.journal.number
2  
dc.journal.pagination
369-387  
dc.journal.pais
Argentina  
dc.journal.ciudad
Buenos Aires  
dc.description.fil
Fil: Canet Juric, Lorena. Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata. Facultad de Psicología. Instituto de Psicología Basica, Aplicada y Tecnología. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas. Centro Científico Tecnológico Conicet - Mar del Plata. Instituto de Psicología Basica, Aplicada y Tecnología; Argentina  
dc.description.fil
Fil: Andrés, María Laura. Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata. Facultad de Psicología. Instituto de Psicología Basica, Aplicada y Tecnología. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas. Centro Científico Tecnológico Conicet - Mar del Plata. Instituto de Psicología Basica, Aplicada y Tecnología; Argentina  
dc.description.fil
Fil: García Coni Bosch, Ana Virginia. Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata. Facultad de Psicología. Instituto de Psicología Basica, Aplicada y Tecnología. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas. Centro Científico Tecnológico Conicet - Mar del Plata. Instituto de Psicología Basica, Aplicada y Tecnología; Argentina  
dc.description.fil
Fil: Richard's, Maria Marta. Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata. Facultad de Psicología. Instituto de Psicología Basica, Aplicada y Tecnología. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas. Centro Científico Tecnológico Conicet - Mar del Plata. Instituto de Psicología Basica, Aplicada y Tecnología; Argentina  
dc.description.fil
Fil: Burin, Debora Ines. Universidad de Buenos Aires. Facultad de Psicología; Argentina. Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas; Argentina  
dc.journal.title
Interdisciplinaria  
dc.relation.alternativeid
info:eu-repo/semantics/altIdentifier/url/http://www.ciipme-conicet.gov.ar/ojs/index.php?journal=interdisciplinaria&page=article&op=view&path%5B%5D=345  
dc.relation.alternativeid
info:eu-repo/semantics/altIdentifier/url/https://dialnet.unirioja.es/servlet/articulo?codigo=6267178